For many years, researchers have examined the mechanisms involved in reading morphologically complex words. It is widely agreed upon that complex words are automatically decomposed into morphemic subunits during the initial stages of visual word recognition (as initially suggested by Taft & Forster, 1975). It has been proposed that the segmentation of morphologically complex words is based on a mechanism that identifies and strips off the affix, which then in turn allows the identification of the stem morpheme. However, this hypothesis has recently been challenged by Grainger and Beyersmann (2017) suggesting that embedded words (rather than affixes) represent the primary reading units that initiate morphological segmentation. The focus of our present study was therefore on the role of the stem, rather than the affix, during early visual word recognition. In particular, our goal was to shed more light on the mechanisms involved in activating words embedded in compound words and compound nonwords.
Evidence obtained with various paradigms suggests that the processing of the constituent lexemes in compound words, such as text and book in textbook, contributes to the overall process of compound word recognition (for a review, see Marelli & Luzzatti, 2012). The masked priming paradigm (Forster & Davis, 1984) has played a key role in revealing the mechanisms involved in recognizing morphologically complex words. By presenting complex prime stimuli very briefly, such that the only visible stimuli are morphologically simple targets (e.g., farmer-FARM), participants are generally unaware of the nature of the priming manipulation, deeming the strategic use of prime-target relations highly unlikely (e.g., Rastle, Davis, Marslen-Wilson, & Tyler, 2000; Rastle, Davis, & New, 2004). Many studies have investigated the processing of affixed words (for reviews, see Amenta & Crepaldi, 2012; Rastle & Davis, 2008), and the results suggest that these are rapidly segmented into stems and affixes at pre-lexical stages of word recognition, independently of semantics. However, very few masked priming studies have investigated the processing of compound words. Compound words differ from derived words in one obvious but fundamental aspect: stems are combined with stems, rather than stems with affixes. Any structural compound priming effects can therefore not be attributed to the traditional affix-stripping hypothesis. Furthermore, practically all of these studies have investigated priming of compound word targets with constituent primes (e.g., farm-farmhouse; Crepaldi, Rastle, Davis, & Lupker, 2013; Duñabeitia, Marín, Avilés, Perea, & Carreiras, 2009; Shoolman & Andrews, 2003), which is the direct opposite of the conditions tested in studies examining non-compound derivations, namely, complex words priming simplex targets (e.g., farmer-farm; Beyersmann, Ziegler, et al., 2016; Rastle & Davis, 2008; Rastle et al., 2000; Rastle et al., 2004). Given the visibility of the compound word target, these studies cannot answer questions concerning the automaticity of morphological processing during visual word recognition.
To our knowledge, only one published study has examined masked priming by compound primes on constituent targets. Fiorentino and Fund-Reznicek (2009) found equivalent priming from transparent (teacup) and opaque (honeymoon) compound words on both the first and second constituent (teacup-TEA/teacup-CUP). No priming was found in the non-compound control condition (window-WIN). These results suggest that compound words are automatically segmented into their morphemic constituents without any influence from semantics, and are therefore consistent with much prior masked priming research on affixed words. Importantly however, the mechanism that has been described to account for pseudo-affixed word priming effects (e.g., corner-corn; Rastle et al., 2004), suggesting that affixes are rapidly chunked/stripped-off, can obviously not account for the type of structural compound priming effects observed by Fiorentino and Fund-Reznicek (2009). Instead, these findings indicate that the activation of embedded stems is only successful if the written word can be exhaustively decomposed into its morphemic consituents (as is the case tea-cup and honey-moon) and so such activation would not occur with non-compound words such as window. But what precisely is the mechanism that prevents the activation of the embedded word win in window?
One theory that provides an answer to this important question is the edge-aligned embedded word activation theory proposed by Grainger and Beyersmann (2017), by which embedded words are activated at both ‘edges’ of the letter string (i.e., when the string begins or ends with the embedded word). The activation of embedded words is based on a purely non-morphological process of mapping input letters onto existing whole-word representations in the orthographic lexicon. Grainger and Beyersmann’s account predicts that the reading system rapidly identifies embedded words independently of morphological structure (e.g., tea and cup in teacup, honey and moon in honeymoon, and win in window). Due to lateral inhibition between the whole word (e.g., teacup, honeymoon, window, etc.) and the embedded words (e.g., tea, cup honey, moon, win, etc.), the activation of the embedded words is initially hindered. However, due to the principle of full decomposition, the identification of embedded words is facilitated when the whole letter string can be completely divided into constituent morphemes. For example, teacup can be divided into tea and cup, honeymoon can be divided into honey and moon, whereas for window the principle of full decomposition fails. Grainger and Beyersmann (2017) propose that the principle of full decomposition provides a boost in activation to the embedded word that helps overcome the lateral inhibition between the embedded word and the whole letter string, which explains why significant priming is observed in the teacup and honeymoon conditions, but not in the window condition.
In the present study, we build on Fiorentino and Fund-Reznicek’s initial work in a further exploration of the mechanisms involved in reading compound words. If it is true that the morphological segmentation of compound words is uniquely based on structural information, one would expect to observe priming not only from real compound word primes (textbook), but also compound-nonword primes (pilebook). Since compound-nonwords cannot be mapped onto an existing representation in the lexicon, any evidence for priming in these conditions would therefore provide clear-cut evidence for pre-lexical activation of embedded words. Moreover, if it is true that ‘edge-alignedness’ is a key factor determining the activation of embedded words (Grainger & Beyersmann, 2017), we would expect significant and equivalent priming for words when embedded in initial and final string position (Experiment 1), but not when embedded in mid-string position (Experiment 2).
An additional goal of our study was to compare priming from compound-nonwords (pilebook) and non-compound nonwords (pimebook). The embedded word activation hypothesis of Grainger and Beyersmann (2017) predicts that the activation of the embedded word is not hindered by the lexical representation of the nonword prime in these conditions. That is, one would expect to see priming not only in the compound nonword condition (pilebook- BOOK), but also in the non-compound nonword condition (pimebook- BOOK). Since pimebook is not lexically represented, it does not compete with the lexical representation of the embedded word book and is therefore able to facilitate priming in this condition. Indeed, several recent masked priming studies have reported comparable magnitudes of priming to words embedded in affixed and non-affixed nonword primes (Beyersmann, Casalis, Ziegler, & Grainger, 2015; Beyersmann, Cavalli, Casalis, & Colé, 2016; Beyersmann & Grainger, 2017; Hasenäcker, Beyersmann, & Schroeder, 2016; Morris, Porter, Grainger, & Holcomb, 2011). Beyersmann, Casalis, et al. (2015), for instance, reported significant priming effects not only when the target was preceded by a suffixed word prime (banker-BANK) or a suffixed nonword prime (bankify-BANK), but also when it was preceded by a non-suffixed nonword prime (bankord-BANK). Moreover, when participants were split into two groups depending on their individual vocabulary and spelling proficiencies, it was found that high proficiency participants showed robust priming in all three prime conditions, whereas low proficiency participants showed significantly reduced non-suffixed priming compared to the two suffixed conditions (see also Andrews & Lo, 2013, for comparable findings). Given this influence of language proficiency on morphological priming effects, we also measured the language proficiency of the participants in the present study.
Importantly, embedded word priming effects have not always been supported by the literature. For instance, an influential study by (Longtin & Meunier, 2005) reported greater priming from suffixed nonword primes (bankify-BANK) than non-suffixed nonword primes (bankord-BANK), thus providing evidence in favor of a morpho-orthographic segmentation mechanism that is insensitive to semantics but sensitive to surface morphological structure (similar results, although less clear cut, are also reported by McCormick, Rastle, & Davis, 2009). These findings conflict with the above reported embedded word priming studies, and thus further highlight the need for a close examination of the underlying embedded word activation mechanisms.
Eighty students from Macquarie University, all English native speakers, participated for course credit. Prior to testing, all participants received information about the study and signed a written consent.
We selected a list of 52 compound words (textbook) from the CELEX lexical database (Baayen, Piepenbrock, & van Rijn, 1993), using a number of selection criteria. All compound-constituents were either nouns, verbs or adjectives (we excluded anybody) and always mono-morphemic (we excluded glassmaking, as well as pseudo-affixed constituents such as the butter of buttermilk and the body of bodyguard). Compound words were always transparent (e.g., we excluded partially opaque compounds like keyboard, where the most frequent usage of key is door-key). The compound’s second constituent never appeared as a first constituent (since we used textbook, we excluded bookshelf).
Based on the compound words, we created two stimulus sets (Appendix A). In Set 1, compound primes were paired with a target that was the compound’s second constituent (textbook-BOOK). In Set 2, compound primes were paired with a target that was the compound’s first constituent (textbook-TEXT). Compound word primes were nearly identical in Sets 1 and 2, except that 5 compound words had to be replaced in Set 2 to avoid target repetition. Target words of Sets 1 and 2 were matched on word frequency, length and orthographic neighborhood (descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1).
Table 1
Descriptive statistics for primes and targets used in Experiment 1.
word initial embeddings | |||
---|---|---|---|
2nd constituent of compound word | 2nd constituent of compound nonword | target word | |
word frequency | 196.82 | 137.13 | 145.69 |
number of syllables | 1.10 | 1.06 | 1.10 |
number of phonemes | 3.37 | 3.33 | 3.27 |
number of letters | 4.27 | 4.25 | 4.13 |
orthographic neighbourhood | 8.73 | 8.02 | 7.40 |
phonological neighbourhood | 19.44 | 17.04 | 16.50 |
word final embeddings | |||
1st constituent of compound word | 1st constituent of compound nonword | target word | |
word frequency | 139.19 | 118.95 | 172.31 |
number of syllables | 1.10 | 1.04 | 1.08 |
number of phonemes | 3.21 | 3.29 | 3.33 |
number of letters | 4.12 | 4.12 | 4.21 |
orthographic neighbourhood | 7.06 | 7.88 | 8.92 |
phonological neighbourhood | 16.79 | 15.71 | 19.38 |
In addition to the compound words, we selected 52 compound-nonwords, 52 non-compound nonwords, and 52 unrelated nonwords. In Set 1, compound-nonwords were created by replacing the first constituent of the compound word with a novel word constituent (pilebook-BOOK), non-compounds by replacing 1–2 letters within the first constituent of the compound word (pimebook-BOOK), and unrelated nonwords were created by replacing the second constituent of the compound prime with an orthographically unrelated mono-morphemic constituent (textjail-BOOK). In Set 2, primes were created by replacing the second constituents, using the same principles as in Set 1 (textpile-TEXT/textpime-TEXT/jailbook-TEXT). Constituents of compound words and compound-nonwords were of the exact same length (textbook vs. pilebook). Constituents of compound-nonword primes were selected such that they never occurred in first or second constituent position in real compound primes. Constituents of compound words and compound nonwords were matched on frequency, number of letters, number of phonemes, number of syllables, orthographic neighborhood, and phonological neighbourhood (see Table 1). All item specific variables were retrieved from the LEXIQUE database (New, Pallier, Brysbaert, & Ferrand, 2004).
For the purpose of the lexical decision task, we included 52 nonword targets (NESH, NUNE, etc.), which were orthographically legal and pronounceable and matched on length to the real-word targets. Each nonword target was preceded by four different primes, which were structured in the same way as the primes preceding the word targets. In Set 1, primes were created by replacing the second constituents of the compound word, compound-nonwords, and non-compound conditions with the nonword target (textnesh-NESH/pilenesh-NESH/pimenesh-NESH), compared to an unrelated control (textbrav-NESH). In Set 2, primes were created by replacing the first constituents (nunebook-NUNE/nunepile-NUNE/nunepime-NUNE/wostbook-NUNE). To avoid target repetition, we created four counterbalanced lists.
Stimuli were presented in the centre of an LED computer screen using DMDX software (Forster & Forster, 2003). Each trial consisted of a 500-ms forward mask of hash keys, then a 50-ms prime in lowercase, then the uppercase target. The target remained present until the response was made or until 3 seconds had elapsed. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible.
In addition to the masked primed lexical decision task, each participant was assessed with a vocabulary, spelling proficiency, reading efficiency, and a reading proficiency test.
Vocabulary. The Shipley-2 (Shipley, Gruber, Martin, & Klein, 2009) was used as a vocabulary test, consisting of 40 items which increased in difficulty as the test progressed. For each item, a target word was presented in uppercase (TALK), and participants were asked to circle one word out of four alternatives that had a corresponding meaning (draw, eat, speak, sleep). Participants were given up to 10 minutes to complete the 40 items.
Spelling proficiency. The spelling test comprised 50 words (8–10 letters long) selected from Burt and Tate (2002). First, words were verbally presented. Then they were included in a carrier sentence to clarify their meaning, and then repeated. Participants were asked to correctly spell each word.
Reading efficiency. Reading efficiency was assessed using the sight word efficiency subtest and the phonemic decoding subtest of the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE; Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1999), Form A. Both subtests measured the number of words/nonwords participants could name in 45 seconds (for each test), with increasing difficulty as the test progressed. The sight word subtest was administered first, followed by the phonemic decoding subtest. The sight word list included 104 words, and the phonemic subtest 63 nonwords. Participants were asked to read as many words/nonwords as quickly as they could. The score for each subtest was the number of words/nonwords read correctly within 45 seconds.
Both subtests were scored following the protocol outlined by Torgeson et al. (1999). Because the TOWRE is an American normed test, we made some modifications to the scoring guidelines for the phonemic decoding subtest. Specifically, in addition to the Torgeson et al. (1999) protocol, we scored participants’ nonword responses in the phonemic subtest as correct if it was deemed correct following the Australian child comparison data reported by Marinus, Kohnen, and McArthur (2013).
Reading proficiency. An extended version of the Castles and Coltheart Test 2 (Castles et al., 2009) was administered to assess the participants’ ability to sound out words and their whole word recognition ability. The extended test included an additional 45 items that were selected to be challenging for adult readers. Participants read aloud 55 regular words (cat, oust), 55 irregular words (yacht, heir), and 55 nonwords (gop, spogsoub), which were presented one at a time, in a mixed order. The degree of difficulty gradually increased as the task went on.
Lexical decisions to word targets were analysed as follows. Incorrect responses were removed from the reaction time (RT) analysis (2.1% of all data). Inverse RTs (1/RT) were calculated for each participant to correct for RT distribution skew and used throughout the analyses. There were no reaction times smaller than 300 ms or larger than 3000 ms that had to be excluded from the analyses. RTs and error rates are presented in Table 2.
Table 2
Table 2 shows mean lexical decision times (ms) and error rates (%) for word targets in Experiment 1, averaged across participants. Standard errors are shown in parentheses.
Condition | Reaction times | Error Rates | Example |
---|---|---|---|
First Constituent | |||
compound word | 544 (10) | 1.3 (0.5) | textbook-TEXT |
compound-nonword | 558 (11) | 2.5 (0.8) | textpile-TEXT |
non-compound nonword | 557 (11) | 2.5 (0.9) | textpime-TEXT |
unrelated | 569 (10) | 2.7 (0.8) | jailbook-TEXT |
Second Constituent | |||
compound word | 545 (10) | 1.5 (0.5) | textbook-BOOK |
compound-nonword | 557 (12) | 1.2 (0.4) | pilebook-BOOK |
non-compound nonword | 548 (9) | 3.1 (0.9) | pimebook-BOOK |
unrelated | 569 (14) | 1.9 (0.5) | textjail-BOOK |
We used linear mixed-effects modelling to perform the main analyses (Baayen, 2008; Baayen, Davidson, & Bates, 2008). Fixed and random effects were only included if they significantly improved the model’s fit in a backward stepwise model selection procedure. Models were selected using chi-squared log-likelihood ratio tests with regular maximum likelihood parameter estimation. Trial order was included to control for longitudinal task effects such as fatigue or habituation. To assess whether the obtained effects were modulated by individual differences in vocabulary, spelling or reading proficiency, the vocabulary, spelling and reading proficiency scores were standardized. A composite measure of reading (ReadZ) was calculated by averaging the standardized scores of the two reading tests. The composite ReadZ measures and standardized spelling (SpellZ) and vocabulary scores (VocabZ) were highly correlated (ReadZ vs. SpellZ: r = .684; ReadZ vs. VocabZ: r = .480; SpellZ vs. VocabZ: r = .454). Thus, the first measure of individual differences used was one that captured the common variance between ReadZ, SpellZ and VocabZ (LanguageProficiencyZ), and which had high positive correlations with all three measures (reading: r = .908; spelling: r = .842; vocabulary: r = .737). Three additional measures of individual differences were used: one that captured the difference between ReadZ and SpellZ (ReadSpellDiffZ), one that captured the difference between ReadZ and VocabZ (ReadVocabDiffZ), and one that captured the difference between SpellZ and VocabZ (SpellVocabDiffZ). ReadSpellDiffZ showed opposite directions of relationship with reading (r = .114) and spelling (r = –.647). ReadVocabDiffZ showed opposite directions of relationship with reading (r = .312) and vocabulary (r = –.684). SpellVocabDiffZ showed opposite directions of relationship with spelling (r = .522) and vocabulary (r = –.522).
A linear mixed-effects model, as implemented in the lme4 package (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2014) in the statistical software R (Version 3.0.3; R Development Core Team, 2008), was created with seven fixed effects factors (primetype: compound word, compound-nonword, non-compound nonword, unrelated; LanguageProficiencyZ; ReadSpellDiffZ; ReadVocabDiffZ; SpellVocabDiffZ; embedded word position: first constituent, second constituent; trial order), their interactions, and two random effects factors (random intercepts for subjects and items). All continuous variables were centred (i.e. trial order, LanguageProficiencyZ, ReadSpellDiffZ, ReadVocabDiffZ, and SpellVocabDiffZ). Both here and throughout, values with regard to p were determined using the package lmerTest (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, 2014). The model was refitted after excluding data-points whose standardized residuals were larger than 2.5 in absolute value (2.6%; see Baayen, 2008). RT analyses revealed a significant priming effect in the compound word condition (t = 6.63, p < .001), in the compound-nonword condition (t = 3.48, p < .001) and in the non-compound nonword condition (t = 4.22, p <. 001), relative to the unrelated control. Moreover, priming was significantly greater in the compound word than in the compound-nonword or non-compound nonword conditions (t = 2.39, p = .017; t = 6.63, p <. 001), but there was no significant difference between priming in the compound-nonword and non-compound nonword conditions (t = 0.75, p = .451). LanguageProficiencyZ, SpellVocabDiffZ, and ReadVocabDiffZ did not yield any significant effects or interactions. However, RT analyses revealed that priming in the compound word condition was significantly modulated by SpellReadDiffZ (Figure 1), indicating that participants whose reading skills were more proficient than their spelling skills showed more priming in the compound word condition relative to the unrelated condition (t = 2.30, p = .021). There was also a significant effect of trial order (t = 2.47, p = .014). No other effects were significant.
Priming effects for targets preceded by compound word, compound-nonword, and non-compound nonword primes (relative to the unrelated control condition), as a function of individual differences in reading and spelling proficiency. Positive proficiency scores represent individuals who are better readers than spellers. Negative proficiency scores represent individuals who are better spellers than readers.
Error analyses followed the same logic as the RT analyses. We applied a binomial variance assumption to the trial-level binary data using the function glmer as part of the R-package lme4. There was a significant effect of trial order (z = 2.83, p = .005) and a significant effect of SpellVocabDiffZ (z = 2.11, p = .035), suggesting that participants with higher spelling than vocabulary skills produced fewer errors. No other effects were significant.
The results of Experiment 1 are clear-cut. Priming is obtained independently of whether the embedded target is accompanied by a real morpheme (e.g., textbook-BOOK and pilebook-BOOK) or by a non-morphemic ending (e.g., pimebook-BOOK), and independently of position (i.e. in initial or final string position: textbook-BOOK/textbook-TEXT). The priming effect seen in the pimebook-BOOK condition is consistent with previous embedded word priming studies (Beyersmann et al., 2015; Beyersmann, Cavalli, et al., 2016; Beyersmann & Grainger, 2017; Hasenäcker et al., 2016; Morris et al., 2011). Moreover, greater priming was obtained in the compound word condition relative to the two nonword prime conditions, which was particularly pronounced in individuals who were better readers than spellers (we return to this point in the General Discussion). Most importantly, the results of Experiment 1 suggest that the activation of embedded words is based on an entirely non-morphological process, which is consistent with Grainger and Beyersmann’s (2017) embedded word activation hypothesis. Embedded words are activated simply by mapping the letters of the input string onto existing whole-word representations in the orthographic lexicon, independently of morphological structure.
However, if it is true that the activation of embedded words is based on a clearly non-morphological process, one crucial question to ask is why Fiorentino and Fund-Reznicek (2009) failed to observe priming in the window-WIN condition? We hypothesise that the absence of priming in this condition can be explained by lateral inhibition between the lexical representations of window and win. Of course, there would also be lateral inhibition between opaque compound words (e.g., butterfly) and their embedded words (e.g., butter). Here, however, the presence of the second constituent (e.g., fly) would boost the activation of the first constituent (butter) and therefore produce priming (Grainger & Beyersmann, 2017).
The goal of Experiment 2 was to further manipulate the position of the embedded words. In particular, our goal was to more closely examine the principle of ‘edge-alignedness’ of embedded word processing. Grainger and Beyersmann (2017) proposed that embedded words receive the greatest bottom-up support when they are edge-aligned, because stems occur less often internally and because edge-aligned words benefit from having a space next to one letter which can be used as an anchor point during orthographic processing (Fischer-Baum, Charny, & McCloskey, 2011). Experiment 2 was designed to test this hypothesis.
The aim of Experiment 2 was to compare edge-aligned embedded word priming (e.g., pimebook-BOOK), to a mid-embedded condition (e.g., pibookme-BOOK) and an outer-embedded condition (e.g., bopimeok-BOOK). If the activation of embedded words is simply due to lower-level orthographic letter-to-word mappings, we would expect that the amount of priming should be determined by the predicted amount of orthographic overlap between prime and target. The predicted amount of orthographic overlap differs depending on different orthographic coding schemes (cf. Match Calculator software, Version 1.9, programmed by Colin J. Davis).1 The binary open-bigram model (Grainger & van Heuven, 2003) generates comparable proportions of orthographic overlap for the edge-aligned and mid conditions, and relatively less overlap in the outer condition. This model thus predicts significantly less priming in the outer condition compared to the edge-aligned and mid conditions. The overlap open-bigram model (Grainger, Granier, Farioli, Van Assche, & van Heuven, 2006), the SERIOL model (Whitney & Cornelissen, 2008), and the SOLAR model (Davis, 2010) generate comparable proportions of orthographic overlap in all three conditions, and therefore predict comparable amounts of priming across all three conditions. However, if it is true that the reading system gives priority to letters that are contiguous with either the first or the last letter of the string (Grainger & Beyersmann, 2017), we would expect to see significantly more priming in the edge-aligned condition compared to the mid and outer control conditions, with comparable magnitudes of priming between the mid and outer conditions.
Forty-seven students from the University of New South Wales, all English native speakers, participated for course credit.
We used the exact same non-compound nonwords with word final edge-aligned embeddings (e.g., pimebook-BOOK) as in Experiment 1 (see Appendix A). In addition, we created a mid-embedded condition (e.g., pibookme-BOOK) and an outer-embedded condition (e.g., bopimeok-BOOK). Some primes were slightly changed to avoid having letters of the target being repeated in the second constituent of the prime. Finally, we used the items of the unrelated control condition of Experiment 1, but the first constituent was replaced with the non-morphemic first constituent of the non-compound prime (e.g., fraltdraft instead of motordraft). All items are listed in Appendix B.
The procedure of the masked priming task was identical to the one used in Experiment 1. Since Experiment 1 showed no evidence for the influence of vocabulary, reading and morphological awareness on masked non-compound priming, we did not assess individual proficiency measures in Experiment 2.
Lexical decisions to word targets were analysed as follows. Incorrect responses were removed from the reaction time (RT) analysis (5.4% of all data). Inverse RTs (1/RT) were calculated for each participant to correct for RT distribution skew and used throughout the analyses. Reaction times smaller than 300 ms or larger than 3000 ms were excluded from the analyses (1.2% of the data). The data from two participants and one item were excluded, because error rates were above 20%. RTs and error rates are presented in Table 3.
Table 3
Table 3 shows mean lexical decision times (ms) and error rates (%) for word targets in Experiment 2, averaged across participants. Standard errors are shown in parentheses.
Condition | Reaction times | Error Rates | Example |
---|---|---|---|
edge-aligned | 552 (12) | 2.8 (0.9) | pimebook-BOOK |
outer | 561 (11) | 6.8 (1.1) | bopimeok-BOOK |
mid | 563 (12) | 5.2 (1.0) | pibookme-BOOK |
unrelated | 564 (12) | 5.6 (0.9) | pimejail-BOOK |
As in Experiment 1, we used linear mixed-effects modelling to perform the main analyses (Baayen, 2008; Baayen et al., 2008). A linear mixed-effects model was created with two fixed effects factors (primetype: edge-aligned, mid, outer, unrelated; trial order) and two random effects factors (random intercepts for subjects and items). All continuous variables were centred. The model was refitted after excluding data-points with standardized residuals larger than 2.5 in absolute value (2.5%; see Baayen, 2008). RT analyses revealed a significant priming effect in the edge-aligned embedded word condition (t = 2.44, p = .015), but not in the mid-embedded condition (t = 0.24, p = .811) or the outer embedded condition (t = 0.38, p = .708), relative to the unrelated control. Moreover, the results revealed that priming was significantly greater in the edge-aligned condition than in the mid or outer conditions (t = 2.69, p = .007; t = 2.05, p = .040), but there was no significant difference between priming in the mid and outer conditions (t = 0.61, p = .539). There was also a significant effect of trial order (t = 2.96, p = .003). No other effects were significant.
Error analyses revealed a similar pattern as the RT analyses. There was a significant priming effect in the edge-aligned embedded word condition (z = 2.05, p = .041), but not in the mid-embedded condition (z = 0.14, p = .888) or the outer embedded condition (t = 1.24, p = .216), relative to the unrelated control. Moreover, the results revealed that priming was significantly greater in the edge-aligned condition than in the mid or outer conditions (z = 2.17, p = .030; z = 3.17, p = .002), but there was no significant difference between priming in the mid and outer conditions (z = 1.10, p = .271). No other effects were significant.
The results of Experiment 2 confirm the second constituent priming effects found with non-compound nonword primes (e.g., pimebook-BOOK) in Experiment 1. Crucially, the significant priming effects found in this condition in Experiment 2 contrast with the non-significant priming effects seen with primes that share the same number of letters with targets but are either edge-aligned but non-contiguous (e.g., bopimeok-BOOK) or contiguous but non-edge-aligned (e.g., pibookme-BOOK). The first result is evidence that orthographic overlap in itself is not sufficient to generate priming effects, and the second result is evidence that having the target word embedded in the prime is not sufficient to generate priming effects. The complete set of results points to edge-alignment and lexical status of the letters shared by prime and target as two key factors driving the priming effects seen in Experiment 1.
The primary goal of our study was to investigate embedded word activation processes operating during the processing of compound words and compound nonwords by using masked priming combined with the lexical decision task. In Experiment 1, significant priming effects relative to unrelated primes were observed for compound word primes (textbook-BOOK/textbook-TEXT), compound nonword primes (pilebook-BOOK/textpile-TEXT), and non-compound nonword primes (pimebook-BOOK/textpime-TEXT), and these priming effects did not differ for the first and second constituents. In Experiment 2, significant priming effects were found in the edge-aligned embedded word condition (e.g., pimebook-BOOK), but not in the mid-embedded condition (e.g., pibookme-BOOK) or the outer-embedded condition (e.g., bopimeok-BOOK).
The first key finding of Experiment 1 is the equivalent priming seen for compound nonword primes (pilebook-BOOK) and non-compound nonword primes (pimebook-BOOK). This replicates the embedded word priming effects previously obtained with derived nonword primes (e.g., flexify vs. flexint; Beyersmann et al., 2015; Morris et al., 2011) and thus points to the importance of embedded word activation mechanisms that operate independently of whether the embedded word occurs in combination with a real word (book in pilebook) or a nonword (book in pimebook). Contrary to Beyersmann et al. (2015), however, we failed to observe any influence of language proficiency on the non-compound nonword priming effects. Importantly, Experiment 2 demonstrates that priming is only obtained for edge-aligned embedded words (e.g., book in pimebook) and not for mid-embedded (e.g., pibookme) or outer-embedded positions (e.g., bopimeok), suggesting that the reading system prioritizes activation of embedded words in edge-aligned position. A straightforward explanation for the important role of edge-alignedness during printed word processing is that spaces surrounding written words may provide privileged anchor points for letter-word mappings (for a more detailed outline of this proposal, see Grainger & Beyersmann, 2017; see also Fischer-Baum et al., 2001, for a related proposal of the “both-edges” coding scheme).
The second key finding is that the magnitude of priming was the same whether the target word was the first or the second constituent of the prime (textbook-BOOK vs. textbook-TEXT). This result is compatible with previous studies investigating compound word processing (Crepaldi et al., 2013; Duñabeitia, Laka, Perea, & Carreiras, 2009; Duñabeitia, Marín, et al., 2009; Libben, Gibson, Yoon, & Sandra, 2003; Monsell, 1985; Sandra, 1990; Shoolman & Andrews, 2003; Zwitserlood, 1994), and suggests that both constituents of a compound word contribute equally to lexical access (but see Taft & Forster, 1976; Taft, Xu, & Li, 2017, who suggest that the first constituent is more important). Unlike prefixes and suffixes, embedded words are not subject to positional constraints, and this can explain why the identification of stem morphemes is position independent (Crepaldi et al., 2013). This finding constitutes important evidence against any sequential beginning-to-end processing bias that might influence complex word recognition (for converging evidence, see also Bowers, Davis, & Hanley, 2005). Our results also converge with previous evidence for embedded word activation mechanisms in monomorphemic nonwords (e.g., wish in dwish; Davis & Taft, 2005; Taft et al., 2017). In line with our present findings, Taft et al. (2017) suggest that edge-alignedness is an important factor underlying embedded word processing (i.e., initial and final consonants have priority in assigning letters to their position), whereas priming is not expected to arise for a mid- or outer-embedded words because of the disruption to the onset + vowel + coda structure of the target.
Experiment 1 revealed a third key result, namely that greater priming was obtained in the compound word condition relative to the two nonword prime conditions. This suggests that the whole-word representation of the compound word prime contributed to priming effects. The fact that Fiorentino and Fund-Reznicek (2009) found no difference between transparent and opaque compound word primes suggests that the difference between compound word and compound-nonword primes seen in our study is driven by whole-word form representations of the compound word and not by morpho-semantic representations. However, our observation that priming in the compound word condition was particularly pronounced in those participants who were better readers than spellers (Figure 1), suggests that future research should examine the role of language proficiency in determining the relative size of transparent and opaque compound word priming before ruling out a role for morpho-semantic representations. Highly relevant with respect to this possibility are the results reported by Andrews and Lo (2013), who found that individuals with higher proficiency in spelling relative to vocabulary showed stronger priming from opaque derived word primes (corner-CORN). In line with Andrews and Lo (2013), our present findings suggest that it is participants’ relative spelling proficiency that modulates priming. In other words, those individuals who were relatively poor spellers tend to rely more heavily on whole-word processing, which was also the case in Andrews and Lo (2013).
Our results concerning the effects of language proficiency are in line with the hypothesis that participants who are better readers than spellers place more emphasis on direct access to whole-word orthographic representations from print compared with morpho-orthographic segmentation processes. These participants would therefore benefit more from the lexical representation of real compound words (textbook), presumably because they would be particularly proficient in rapidly mapping input letter strings onto their corresponding whole-word representations. On the other hand, those participants who were better spellers than readers would be less efficient in mapping a complete letter string onto its whole-word representation and therefore rely to a greater extent on morpho-orthographic segmentation mechanisms.
Importantly, the present data provide further support for embedded word activation mechanisms operating at the level of whole-word representations. More precisely, our results allow us to rule-out an explanation of embedded word priming effects in terms of lower-level letter overlap between the prime and the target. If orthographic overlap, independently of whether the overlapping letters form a word or not, were the factor driving embedded word priming effects, then we should have observed similar priming from the non-contiguous primes (e.g., bopimeok-BOOK) in Experiment 2. Of course, it could be argued that having non-contiguous orthographic overlap is not an appropriate comparison, and that a more appropriate comparison might involve contiguous letters in so-called partial primes (e.g., pimehant-ELEPHANT).2 Although we agree that future experimentation should investigate this possibility, we would also point out that failure to find a partial priming effect in these conditions would not provide conclusive evidence that embedded word priming is lexically driven, since the partial prime shares four letters out of eight with the target, whereas the embedded word prime shares four letters out of four. On the other hand, finding significant priming with such partial primes would force us to re-consider our word-based interpretation of the present constituent priming effects. Finally, it has been shown that greater masked priming effects are observed for embedded words with many morphological family members compared to embedded words with no morphological family members (Beyersmann & Grainger, 2017), which is also suggestive of a lexical locus of the embedded word priming effect.
Taken together, the present findings, in addition to evidence obtained from prior research using different paradigms (e.g., Andrews, Miller, & Rayner, 2004; Hyönä, Bertram, & Pollatsek, 2004; Pollatsek, Hyönä, & Bertram, 2000; Zwitserlood, 1994), are consistent with parallel dual-route models that allow simultaneous access to both whole-word representations of compound words and the representations of their embedded morphemes (Baayen & Schreuder, 1999; Bertram & Hyönä, 2003; Diependaele, Sandra, & Grainger, 2009). However, it is also possible that the additional facilitation in the real compound condition in Experiment 1 comes from morphologically-mediated activation of the whole-word representation, rather than from a parallel whole-word processing route. Most importantly, our data suggest that embedded words (book) are activated when embedded at the edges of the letter string, independently of whether they occur in combination with a real morpheme (pilebook) or a non-morphemic constituent (pimebook). Our findings therefore point to the importance of embedded word activation mechanisms during complex word processing, in line with Grainger and Beyersmann’s (2017) recent proposal.
Could JoC please help me to make the data available on their website? If this is possible, I will provide the data in excel format via email, so that JoC can make these available online.
The Additional file for this article can be found as follows:
Data Files 1 and 2Primes and targets used in Experiments 1 and 2. DOI: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5797338
1This application is available at Colin Davis’ website: http://www.pc.rhul.ac.uk/staff/c.davis/utilities/matchcalc/index.htm.
This research was supported by the Brain and Language Research Institute (BLRI, ANR-11-LABX-0036) and the Institute of Convergence ILCB (ANR-16-CONV-0002). It has benefited from support from the French government, managed by the French National Agency for Research (ANR-15-FRAL-0003-01) and the Excellence Initiative of Aix-Marseille University (A*MIDEX). We thank Nicholas Badcock, Saskia Kohnen, and Eva Marinus for providing access to the extended version of the Castles and Coltheart Test 2, and Cornelia Van Scherpenberg for her help with data collection.
The authors have no competing interests to declare.
Amenta, S. and Crepaldi, D. (2012). Morphological processing as we know it: an analytical review of morphological effects in visual word identification. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 1–12, DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00232
Andrews, S. and Lo, S. (2013). Is morphological priming stronger for transparent than opaque words? It depends on individual differences in spelling and vocabulary. Journal of Memory & Language 68: 279–296, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.12.001
Andrews, S., Miller, B. and Rayner, K. (2004). Eye movements and morphological segmentation of compound words: There is a mouse in mousetrap. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 16: 285–311, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440340000123
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511801686
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J. and Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59: 390–412, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R. and van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM).
Baayen, R. H. and Schreuder, R. (1999). War and peace: Morphemes and full forms in a noninteractive activation parallel dual-route model. Brain and Language 68(1–2): 27–32, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1999.2069
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. and Walker, S. (2014). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-5. Retrieved from: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
Bertram, R. and Hyönä, J. (2003). The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds. Journal of Memory & Language 48: 615–634, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00539-9
Beyersmann, E., Casalis, S., Ziegler, J. C. and Grainger, J. (2015). Language proficiency and morpho-orthographic segmentation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22: 1054–1061, DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0752-9
Beyersmann, E., Cavalli, E., Casalis, S. and Colé, P. (2016). Embedded Stem Priming Effects in Prefixed and Suffixed Pseudowords. Scientific Studies of Reading 20(3): 220–230, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2016.1140769
Beyersmann, E. and Grainger, J. (2017). Support from the morphological family when unembedding the stem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Advance online publication.
Beyersmann, E., Ziegler, J. C., Castles, A., Coltheart, M., Kezilas, Y. and Grainger, J. (2016). Morpho-orthographic segmentation without semantics. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23: 533–539, DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0927-z
Bowers, J. S., Davis, C. J. and Hanley, D. A. (2005). Automatic semantic activation of embedded words: Is there a “hat” in “that”?. Journal of Memory & Language 52: 131–143, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.09.003
Burt, J. and Tate, H. (2002). Does a reading lexicon provide orthographic representations for spelling?. Journal of Memory and Language 46: 518–543, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2818
Castles, A., Coltheart, M., Larsen, L., Jones, P., Saunders, S. and McArthur, G. (2009). Assessing the basic components of reading: A revision of the Castles and Coltheart test with new norms. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties 14(1): 67–88, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/19404150902783435
Crepaldi, D., Rastle, K., Davis, C. J. and Lupker, S. J. (2013). Seeing Stems Everywhere: Position-Independent Identification of Stem Morphemes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 39(2): 510–525, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029713
Davis, C. J. (2010). The Spatial Coding Model of Visual Word Identification. Psychological Review 117: 713, 758.DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019738
Davis, C. J. and Taft, M. (2005). More words in the neighborhood: Interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 12(5): 904–910, DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196784
Diependaele, K., Sandra, D. and Grainger, J. (2009). Semantic transparency and masked morphological priming: The case of prefixed words. Memory & Cognition 37: 895–908, DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.6.895
Duñabeitia, J. A., Laka, I., Perea, M. and Carreiras, M. (2009). Is Milkman a superhero like Batman? Constituent morphological priming in compound words. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 21(4): 615–640, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440802079835
Duñabeitia, J. A., Marín, A., Avilés, A., Perea, M. and Carreiras, M. (2009). Constituent priming effects: Evidence for preserved morphological processing in healthy old readers. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 21: 283–302, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440802281142
Fiorentino, R. and Fund-Reznicek, E. (2009). Masked morphological priming of compound constituents. The Mental Lexicon 4(2): 159–193, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.4.2.01fio
Fischer-Baum, S., Charny, J. and McCloskey, M. (2011). Both-edges representation of letter position in reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18(6): 1083–1089, DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0160-3
Forster, K. I. and Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 35(1): 116–124, DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195503
Grainger, J. and Beyersmann, E. (2017). Ross, B. H. ed. Edge-aligned embedded word activation initiates morpho-orthographic segmentation. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, : 285–317, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.009
Grainger, J., Granier, J. P., Farioli, F., Van Assche, E. and van Heuven, W. J. B. (2006). Letter position information and printed word perception: The relative-position priming constraint. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance 32(4): 865–884, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.865
Grainger, J. and van Heuven, W. J. B. (2003). Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception In: Bonin, P. ed. The Mental Lexicon. New York: Nova Science, pp. 1–23.
Hasenäcker, J., Beyersmann, E. and Schroeder, S. (2016). Masked morphological priming in German-speaking adults and children: Evidence from response time distributions. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 929.DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00929
Hyönä, J., Bertram, R. and Pollatsek, A. (2004). Are long compound words identified serially via their constituents? Evidence from an eye-movement-contingent display change study. Memory & Cognition 32: 523–532, DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195844
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B. and Christensen, R. H. B. (2014). lmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package): R package version 2.0-6, http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest
Libben, G., Gibson, M., Yoon, Y.-B. and Sandra, D. (2003). Compound fracture: The role of semantic transparency and morphological headedness. Brain and Language 84: 26–43, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00520-5
Longtin, C. M. and Meunier, F. (2005). Morphological decomposition in early visual word processing. Journal of Memory and Language 53: 26–41, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.008
Marelli, M. and Luzzatti, C. (2012). Frequency effects in the processing of Italian nominal compounds: Modulation of headedness and semantic transparency. Journal of Memory & Language 66: 644–664, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.01.003
Marinus, E., Kohnen, S. and McArthur, G. (2013). Australian comparison data for the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE). Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties 18(2): 199–212, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2013.852981
McCormick, S. F., Rastle, K. and Davis, M. H. (2009). Adore-able not adorable? Orthographic underspecification studied with masked repetition priming. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 21: 813–836, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440802366919
Monsell, S. (1985). Repetition and the lexicon In: Ellis, A. W. ed. Progress in the psychology of language. London: Erlbaum, 2pp. 147–195.
Morris, J., Porter, J. H., Grainger, J. and Holcomb, P. J. (2011). Effects of lexical status and morphological complexity in masked priming: An ERP study. Language and Cognitive Processes 26(4–6): 558–599, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.495482
New, B., Pallier, C., Brysbaert, M. and Ferrand, L. (2004). Lexique 2: A new French lexical database. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 36: 516–524, DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195598
Pollatsek, A., Hyönä, J. and Bertram, R. (2000). The role of morphological constituents in reading Finnish compound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26: 820–833, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.26.2.820
Rastle, K. and Davis, M. H. (2008). Morphological decomposition based on the analysis of orthography. Language & Cognitive Processes 23: 942–971, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960802069730
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., Marslen-Wilson, W. and Tyler, L. K. (2000). Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time-course study. Language and Cognitive Processes 15(4–5): 507–537, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960050119689
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H. and New, B. (2004). The broth in my brother’s brothel: Morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 11: 1090–1098, DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196742
R Development Core Team (2008). R: A language and environment for statistical computing: R package version 1.1-5, http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4
Sandra, D. (1990). On the representation and processing of compound words: Automatic access to constituent morphemes does not occur. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 42A: 529–567, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749008401236
Shipley, W. C., Gruber, C. P., Martin, T. A. and Klein, A. M. (2009). Shipley-2 manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Shoolman, N. and Andrews, S. (2003). Racehorses, reindeer, and sparrows: Using masked priming to investigate morphological influences on compound word identification In: Kinoshita, S. and Lupker, S. J. eds. Masked priming: The state of the art. Macquarie monographs in cognitive science. New York, NY, US: Psychology Press, pp. 241–278.
Taft, M. and Forster, K. I. (1975). Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 14: 638–647, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(75)80051-X
Taft, M. and Forster, K. I. (1976). Lexical storage and retrieval of polymorphemic and polysyllabic words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Learning Behavior 15: 607–620, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-5371(76)90054-2
Taft, M., Xu, J. and Li, S. (2017). Letter coding in visual word recognition: The impact of embedded words. Journal of Memory and Language 92: 14–25, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.05.002
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K. and Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin, TX: PRO-ED Publishing, Inc..
Whitney, C. and Cornelissen, P. (2008). SERIOL Reading. Language and Cognitive Processes 23(1): 143–164, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960701579771
Zwitserlood, P. (1994). The role of semantic transparency in the processing and representation of Dutch compounds. Language and Cognitive Processes 9: 341–368, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969408402123