Start Submission Become a Reviewer

Reading: The Time Constant of Attentional Control: Short, Medium and Long (Infinite?)

Download

A- A+
Alt. Display

Commentary

The Time Constant of Attentional Control: Short, Medium and Long (Infinite?)

Authors:

Leonardo Chelazzi ,

Department of Neuroscience, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona; National Institute of Neuroscience, Verona, IT
X close

Elisa Santandrea

Department of Neuroscience, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, IT
X close
How to Cite: Chelazzi, L., & Santandrea, E. (2018). The Time Constant of Attentional Control: Short, Medium and Long (Infinite?). Journal of Cognition, 1(1), 27. DOI: http://doi.org/10.5334/joc.24
280
Views
106
Downloads
12
Citations
  Published on 14 May 2018
 Accepted on 15 Mar 2018            Submitted on 01 Mar 2018

The review article by Jan Theeuwes (2018) mainly focuses on the distinction between top-down attention, which is assumed to be slow, effortful and controlled, and biases in selection stemming from selection history, which are assumed to be fast, effortless and automatic (and typically implicit). This distinction does not imply no role for purely salience-driven, or bottom-up, attention, but the latter topic is less crucial here. The term selection history alludes to the fact that the history of selection episodes, both from the recent and more distant past, can elicit lingering selection biases that are unrelated to top-down control or to the physical salience of objects. One further claim is made in the review, namely, that signals pertaining to the current goals of the individual (top-down), the intrinsic stimulus salience (bottom-up) and selection history biases all converge onto the same priority map(s) of space, wherein the compound signal will determine within a winner-take-all architecture which particular stimulus/location will be selected first.

In our commentary to this interesting and stimulating review paper, we want to draw attention onto three specific points that might fuel discussion, as briefly outlined below.

First, we propose that what the author refers to as “selection history” might be more appropriately defined as experience-dependent attentional control, since previous encounters with a given object and/or a given location must not have involved specific attentional operations (i.e., selection or suppression) to exert an influence on ongoing attentional deployment. In line with this notion, some of the studies carried out over recent years to study the influence of reward on attentional control (for relevant reviews, see Anderson, 2016; Bourgeois et al., 2016; Chelazzi et al., 2013; Failing and Theeuwes, in press) employed task paradigms where stimulus-reward associations were created during an initial learning phase devoid of any obvious selection component (e.g., Rutherford et al., 2010). In this context, it will not be possible to ascribe the observed effects to selection history; rather one might hypothesize that they derive from a changed status of certain stimuli in their brain representation. This raises the interesting possibility that experience might be so effective in inducing plasticity in the brain to even shape the basic architecture of visual processing, at least in the form of representations in primary sensory cortices (e.g., Shuler & Bear, 2006; Stănişor et al., 2013), thus mimicking the neural signatures of true bottom-up effects (see below), as the author himself suggests in the final part of his review, although from a phenomenological, rather than neurophysiological, perspective.

Second, we think that a neurophysiological perspective might be of help to better define the different forms of attentional control the author illustrates. Within this perspective, we propose to define: i) Bottom-up attentional control as a semi-stable, hard-wired property of the human brain, directly reflecting basic computation principles such as local contrast or center-surround antagonism (e.g., Angelucci et al., 2017), that for example makes the individual naturally reactive to items that differ from their surroundings (e.g., Treisman & Gelade, 1980; Yantis & Jonides, 1984; Posner & Petersen, 1990; Theeuwes, 1992); ii) Top-down attentional control as a flexible, goal-oriented and intentional form of attention, which is implemented through the dynamic online reconfiguration of relevant brain networks, likely requiring time-consuming (back) propagation of signals from control regions to target areas responsible for visual processing of the retinal input (Baluch & Itti, 2011); and, finally, iii) Experience-dependent attentional control as an instrumental tool for maximizing fitness to the environment, which is implemented through (more or less dramatic and durable) plastic changes in critical neural substrates.

Third, we would like to make some comments regarding the schematic illustration shown in Figure 1 of the review article by Theeuwes, depicting the integration of three sources of bias within the same priority map(s) of space. Our reflection on this point is threefold. On the one hand, we remark the possibility that different sources for attentional control, or biasing signals, rather than being merged through a simple computation (e.g., linear summation), as suggested by the author, might be weighted differently depending on the given context and task demands. For instance, one might conceive the possibility that, depending on the given circumstances, one or the other source will exert greater influence on the priority map, therefore being the primary determinant of attentional deployment; in other words, as recently suggested by Todd and Manaligod, a context-dependent weighting of different biasing signals for AC might be at place (Todd & Manaligod, 2017). On the other hand, it is still an open question whether a “minus sign”, i.e., the possible existence of de-prioritization signals, only pertains to experience-dependent forms of attentional control, as suggested here, or might be also implemented through top-down attention (and perhaps even through bottom-up factors). Yet another, and final, consideration relates to the possibility that multiple priority maps do exist in the brain, each representing priority of objects and locations in the visual space in relation to a specific motor output of the organism, i.e., as linked to the relevant effector for the given behavioral outcome (e.g., Rizzolatti et al., 1987; Berman & Colby, 2009). In line with this notion, an independent allocation of attentional resources to targets of eye and hand movements was demonstrated at the service of non-coordinated gaze and reaching actions (Jonikaitis & Deubel, 2011). Further evidence bearing on all these issues is required, such that the refinement of the notion of priority map(s) will likely be a very fertile area of investigation in the years to come.

Competing Interests

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

References

  1. Anderson, B. A. (2016). The attention habit: how reward learning shapes attentional selection. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 1369, 24–39. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12957 

  2. Angelucci, A., Bijanzadeh, M., Nurminen, L., Federer, F., Merlin, S., & Bressloff, P. C. (2017). Circuits and Mechanisms for Surround Modulation in Visual Cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci, 40, 425–451. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031418 

  3. Baluch, F., & Itti, L. (2011). Mechanisms of top-down attention. Trends Neurosci, 34, 210–224. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2011.02.003 

  4. Berman, R., & Colby, C. (2009). Attention and active vision. Vision Res, 49, 1233–1248. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2008.06.017 

  5. Bourgeois, A., Chelazzi, L., & Vuilleumier, P. (2016). How motivation and reward learning modulate selective attention. Prog Brain Res, 229, 325–342. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2016.06.004 

  6. Chelazzi, L., Perlato, A., Santandrea, E., & Della Libera, C. (2013). Rewards teach visual selective attention. Vision Res, 85, 58–72. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2012.12.005 

  7. Failing, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2018). Selection History: How reward modulates selectivity of visual attention. Psychon Bull Rev, in press. 

  8. Jonikaitis, D., & Deubel, H. (2011). Independent allocation of attention to eye and hand targets in coordinated eye-hand movements. Psychol Sci, 22, 339–347. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610397666 

  9. Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci, 13, 25–42. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325 

  10. Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umiltà, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–40. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(87)90041-8 

  11. Rutherford, H. J., O’Brien, J. L., & Raymond, J. E. (2010). Value associations of irrelevant stimuli modify rapid visual orienting. Psychon Bull Rev, 17, 536–542. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.4.536 

  12. Shuler, M. G., & Bear, M. F. (2006). Reward timing in the primary visual cortex. Science, 311, 1606–1609. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1123513 

  13. Stănişor, L., van der Togt, C., Pennartz, C. M., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2013). A unified selection signal for attention and reward in primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 110, 9136–9141. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1300117110 

  14. Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Percept Psychophys, 51, 599–606. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211656 

  15. TheeuwesJ. (2018). Visual Selection: Usually Fast and Automatic; Seldom Slow and Volitional. Journal of Cognition, 1(1), 29. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.13 

  16. Todd, R. M., & Manaligod, M. G. M. (2017). Implicit guidance of attention: The priority state space framework. Cortex, pii: S0010-9452(17)30248-4. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.001 

  17. Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G., (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cogn Psychol, 12, 97–136. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5 

  18. Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 10, 601–621. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.601 

comments powered by Disqus